Do humans get lazier when robots help with tasks? – study

Business


Millions of people around the world fear that much-improved robots will replace them at their jobs and make them unemployed. Now that improvements in technology mean that some robots work alongside humans, there is evidence that those humans have learned to see them as team-mates – and teamwork can have negative as well as positive effects on people’s performance. 

People sometimes relax, letting their colleagues do the work instead. This is called “social loafing,” and it’s common where people know their contribution won’t be noticed or they’ve acclimatized to another team member’s high performance. Scientists at the Technical University of Berlin investigated whether humans are in fact loafers when they work with robots.

“Teamwork is a mixed blessing,” said Dr. Dietlind Helene Cymek, first author of the study in Frontiers in Robotics and AI just published under the title “Lean back or lean in? exploring social loafing in human–robot teams.”

Do robots make humans lazy?

Traditionally, robots have worked with little or no interaction with human colleagues for safety reasons. In the automotive sector, for example, the payload and speed of large single-arm robots handling body parts pose a serious risk to human workers. However, there is also an emerging trend to bring humans and robots closer together, both physically and temporally, offering a wealth of new applications, the team wrote.

“Working together can motivate people to perform well but it can also lead to a loss of motivation because the individual contribution is not as visible. We were interested in whether we could also find such motivational effects when the team partner is a robot,” she said. 

Animated image of a robot painting flowers. (credit: PUBLICDOMAINPICTURES.NET)

The scientists tested their hypothesis using a simulated industrial defect-inspection task: looking at circuit boards for errors. The scientists provided images of circuit boards to 42 participants. The circuit boards were blurred, and the sharpened images could only be viewed by holding a mouse tool over them. This allowed the scientists to track participants’ inspection of the board.

Half of the participants were told that they were working on circuit boards that had been inspected by a robot called Panda. Although these participants didn’t work directly with Panda, they had seen the robot and could hear it while they worked. After examining the boards for errors and marking them, all participants were asked to rate their own effort, how responsible for the task they felt, and how they performed.

At first sight, it looked as if Panda’s presence had made no difference – there was no statistically significant difference between the groups in terms of time spent inspecting the circuit boards and the area searched. Participants in both groups rated their feelings of responsibility for the task, effort expended, and performance similarly.

But when the scientists looked more closely at participants’ error rates, they realized that the participants working with Panda were catching fewer defects later in the task, when they’d already seen that Panda had successfully flagged many errors. This could reflect a “looking but not seeing” effect, where people get used to relying on something and engage with it less mentally. Although the participants thought they were paying an equivalent amount of attention, subconsciously they assumed that Panda hadn’t missed any defects.

Participants in both groups inspected almost the entire board surface, took their time searching, and rated their subjective effort as high. However, participants working in a team with the robot found on average 3.3 defects. People working alone found significantly more defects on these 5 occasions – an average of 4.2.

“It is easy to track where a person is looking, but much harder to tell whether that visual information is being sufficiently processed at a mental level,” said Dr. Linda Onnasch, a senior author of the study. The authors warned that this could have safety implications. “In our experiment, the subjects worked on the task for about 90 minutes, and we already found that fewer quality errors were detected when they worked in a team,” she said. “In longer shifts, when tasks are routine and the working environment offers little performance monitoring and feedback, the loss of motivation tends to be much greater. In manufacturing in general, but especially in safety-related areas where double checking is common, this can have a negative impact on work outcomes.”

A limitation of the study is the laboratory setting,” Cymek explained. “To find out how big the problem of loss of motivation is in human-robot interaction, we need to go into the field and test our assumptions in real work environments, with skilled workers who routinely do their work in teams with robots.”







Source link

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *